(Translated by https://www.hiragana.jp/)
Johnson | The Economist
The Wayback Machine - https://web.archive.org/web/20120220163630/http://www.economist.com/blogs/johnson

Johnson

Language

  • Announcements

    The Style Guide returns

    Feb 17th 2012, 18:01 by R.L.G. | NEW YORK

    AT LONG last: The Economist'Style Guide is online again. This is not a brand-new edition—in fact, it's several years old—but it appears in a newly browsable alphabetised format. Check out the introductory essay, or perhaps the entries on Metaphors, Journalese and slang or Overused words to get started, and then page around the letter index pages.  

  • Names

    What's wrong with the "Democrat Party"

    Feb 15th 2012, 17:28 by R.L.G. | NEW YORK

    SOME conservative Americans continue annoying Democrats by calling their party the "Democrat Party", and using "Democrat" in other modifying positions like "the Democrat Senate."  Rush Limbaugh, every liberal's least favourite conservative, said yesterday that

    You've got to understand that everything there, I don't care, Washington Post, New York Times, ABC, CBS, NBC, CNN, MSNBC, LA Times. It's all oriented toward two things: advancing the Democrat Party and whoever runs it -- in this case, Barack Obama -- and, at the same time, defeating, embarrassing and humiliating the Republicans and conservatives.

    Annoyed liberals frequently respond with some version of "Democrat is not an adjective." In this version of the complaint, the problem is grammatical ignorance: the party must be called "the Democratic Party", because "Democratic" is the adjective needed. It is correct in the argument that "Democrat" isn't an adjective. It's wrong in arguing that the "Democrat Party" is ungrammatical.

    That's because this complaint ignores a basic fact: nouns are used in modifier position all the time. People wake up and shut off their alarm clock, walk down the hall stairs, turn on their coffee maker, cook an egg using an egg timer, shave in the bathroom mirror, but on their business suit, grab their car keys and so on. Alarm, hall, coffee, egg, bathroom, business and car are all nouns. (You can tell they haven't become adjectives because you can't say "That timer is egg.")  You've almost certainly used a noun-noun compound today. That nouns are often even used to modify "party" can be seen in the names of the Labour Party, the UK Independence Party, the Constitution Party and many others, as Mark Liberman explained in 2007.

    The real reason "Democrat Party" is wrong is not because it's ungrammatical, but because it's incorrect in another way—the party is simply not named the Democrat Party, but the Democratic Party. Calling it anything else is discourteous. Individuals and institutions may choose their names; this is a basic agreement among users of language in our society. If you've made it clear to me you want to be called "Mike", I don't have the right to call you "Mick" or "Jerkwad", unless I don't mind being called unprintable things by everyone else for being deliberately annoying.

    Advice to Democrats: this is the behaviour of attention-seeking children. Ignore it.

  • The history of recording

    The earliest recorded sounds

    Feb 14th 2012, 22:20 by R.L.G. | NEW YORK

    IT MUST have been excruciating for the National Museum of American History's archivists to have the earliest known recordings of the human voice but not to be able to listen to them. The records, made in the Volta Lab of Alexander Graham Bell in the early 1880s, were too fragile to play. But the Lawrence Berkeley National Laboratory figured out how to scan them optically and retrieve the sound, as described on the museum's website here. Six recordings have been released on YouTube. The most familiar text is this one:

     

    Bell's lab was in competition with that of Thomas Edison, who invented the phonograph that became the standard way of recording sound. The first phonograph recordings available come from 1888, so this amazing work has pushed that boundary back a few years to the early 1880s. 

    Of linguistic note is the accent. "Suffer" has the same lack of rhotic "r" after a vowel that one finds in standard British English today, but not in most American accents. The second is that r's in other positions are noticeably trilled, characteristic of old-timey, hyperarticulated stage voices. The vowels, too, have a British flavour.

    The missing piece that we will never have, unfortunately, is truly spontaneous speech from the era. Linguists know that people talk differently when a recording device is prominent in front of them, and surely even more so when recorded sound was as rare and fantastic as it was in the 1880s. In fact, Candid Microphone, a comedy radio show from the 1940s, is one of the oldest large collections of people being recorded without knowing it. An example can be heard here.  

    But the Volta recordings should nonetheless prove fascinating. The researchers think that they will isolate, over time, the voice of the great Bell himself.

    (Via Language Hat.)

  • Solecisms

    The comedic potential of “literally”

    Feb 14th 2012, 16:13 by F.C. | NEW YORK

    FANS of the American mockumentary show “Parks and Recreation”, created by NBC, have noted with delight the serial abuse of “literally” by Rob Lowe’s character Chris Traeger. He eccentrically pronounces it /ˈlit rə li/ (close to the standard British pronunciation) every time he uses and misuses it, which is often.  

    “I have a resting heart rate of 28 beats per minute. The doctors who studied me said that my heart could, literally, pump jet fuel up into a jet.” A send-up of type-A personalities, Traeger characterizes various foods, people and experiences as literally the best. The comedic value of a character like Traeger comes from the deformative effects his sunny perspective has on his own speech. He expresses delight at the most banal details (“Biking for charity is literally one of my interests on Facebook”). His personality leaves people in awe and often wondering, “Is this guy for real?" (While cold-calling for a telethon Traeger asks, “Is this Deirdre Splatterfork? That is literally the most beautiful name I have ever heard.”)

    It would seem that the more frequently one uses “literally” in everyday speech, the more often one’s sincerity could be called into question. But not always. In one episode, Traeger describes his workout regimen, which is obnoxiously predicated on earth’s literal distance to the moon. His is a world of infinite possibility. "I have run ten miles a day, everyday for 18 years. That’s 65,000 miles. A third of the way to the moon. My goal is to run to the moon.”  In many ways, Traeger is the polar opposite of Aubrey Plaza’s character April Ludgate, the moody intern-cum-secretary whose doe-eyed servings of deadpan are oil to Traeger’s watered-down speech. Unlike Traeger, Ludgate is openly mean and her expressions of sentiment are so rare, they function as unlikely plot devices. 

    Both characters lie, but in different ways. Ludgate lies for her own amusement or lies about her own enjoyment in order to remain emotionally distant. Traeger, the perverted optimist, has trouble acknowledging any sort of trouble. And so when trouble comes, he copes desperately with—what else—"literally". In a spoof of the Anthony Weiner scandal, Rashida Jones’s character, a nurse, is email-bombed by anxious bureaucrats seeking health advice re: their genitalia. Traeger’s response: “Oh my god, your in-box is literally filled with penises!”

    In a more recent episode, Traeger’s character is dumped, a worldview-shaking occurrence for someone with such cheekbones. As his character arc continues, and presumably, his outlook on life becomes wearied, it will be interesting to see whether Traeger’s abuse of the word “literally” abates, or is somehow amended.

    Until then, there are always re-runs.

  • Euphemisms

    Unsatisfactory terminology

    Feb 10th 2012, 19:39 by H.J. | SÃO PAULO

    A sad, sad moment: the scrapping of the Ofsted judgment of “satisfactory”. For readers who live outside Britain, Ofsted is the schools inspectorate, and “satisfactory” has for years been one of the categories in which it puts schools, at least until now. As I noted a couple of years ago in an article for the print edition about education jargon, or “eduspeak”, in this context “satisfactory” means precisely “unsatisfactory”. It was hearing the then head of Ofsted give an interview in which she said that far too many of England’s schools were satisfactory—a statement that presumably provoked bewilderment in many visitors to the country—that inspired my article. 

    I’m sad to see “satisfactory” go, but at least it went out with a bang: the prime minister, David Cameron, responded to the news with the priceless: “I don't want the word ‘satisfactory’ to exist in our education system.”

    Any other examples spring to readers’ minds?

  • Gay marriage

    A right to be called "married" in California

    Feb 8th 2012, 18:11 by R.L.G. | NEW YORK

    YESTERDAY, a California-based federal court nullified Proposition 8, a Californian referendum that stripped away the right of gays to marry in the state. Prop 8 passed in 2008, and has been the subject of hundreds of thousands of words of journalism since. One of the curious facts is that the linguistic nature of Prop 8 has been sidelined. But as John Stokes reminds readers today, it's the central issue. The introduction of gay marriage in California created no new substantive rights: the state already had civil partnerships with the full rights of married couples. Prop 8 did nothing to strip these rights away. And so yesterday's ruling will restore no rights again—except, that is, a linguistic one: the right to hold up a state certificate calling you and your partner "married".

    Do you have a right to be called "married" if the state has recognised your right to every benefit of marriage (in taxation, hospital visits, inheritance and so on)? Or, more narrowly, if someone has decided you have the right to be "married", can voters later strip that title away? Take it away, Judge Reinhardt:

    The People of California may not, consistent with the Federal Constitution, add to their state constitution a provision that has no more practical effect than to strip gays and lesbians of their right to use the official designation that the State and society give to committed relationships, thereby adversely affecting the status and dignity of the members of a disfavored class.

    [Emphasis added.]

    gay-marriage

    The Economist prefers that these things be settled by legislatures—as New York state has done, and Washington state is about to do. When settled by judges, such controversial issues can fester for a long time. But sometimes civil rights, as with those of black Americans before the 1960s, are not respected by democratic majorities, and courts step in. Our position on the rights of gays to marry is well known and 16 years old, so today is a happy day. If yesterday's ruling holds, gays can not only have a wedding, but get "married" in California once again.

  • Language attitudes

    No words for individual rights

    Feb 6th 2012, 17:52 by L.M. | LONDON

    HERE we go again. The idea that language regulates thought is as old as the hills. (See The Economist's debate on the subject here.) But while experts debate the scientific particulars, laymen sometimes put it to such lazy and odious use that Johnson cannot help but respond. In this week’s issue of Mint Lounge—the weekend edition of an Indian business newspaper run in association with the Wall Street Journal—Aakar Patel, a columnist, argues in favour of restricting free speech in India. 

    Mr Patel makes his case using the single most shameful event in 21st century Indian history: the 2002 riots in the state of Gujarat, during which hundreds of Muslims were slaughtered by Hindus as authorities stood aside and watched. The riots started when a train carriage filled with Hindu pilgrims was set ablaze at a station in Gujarat as they returned from a holy site in north India. 58 men, women and children were killed. Hindu mobs retaliated against the local Muslims, spurred on, says Mr Patel, by local newspapers. So free speech must be regulated because Hindi- and Gujarati-speaking Indians will believe anything they read in the papers. 

    In a series of baseless assertions, Mr Patel claims that the reason all Muslims were blamed for the massacre is that Indians have a collective identity and behaviour. Why? How? He doesn’t say. But he adds:

    It is difficult to explain to Indians the wrongness of collective punishment. This is because our identity is collective, and so is our behaviour. The understanding that this is wrong comes mainly to those who speak English. Individuals are more easily produced by English because it opens access to the world outside the tribe. It is able to place us outside the narrow definitions assigned to us by Gujarati and Hindi. [Emphasis mine]

    Where to begin? The absence of a word from language A in language B is often used to prove that the latter lacks the concept. This is easily disproved: Schadenfreude means satisfaction at another’s misfortune. That no single word for this exists in English doesn’t mean English-speaking people are incapable of feeling the emotion. They just take a number of words to express it. That is a flimsy enough argument at the best of times. But Hindi does not lack the word for individual (व्यक्ति, vyakti; the Gujarati word is similar).

    Maybe Mr Patel means individual rights cannot be expressed in Hindi. In that case perhaps he should read the Hindi version of the Universal Declaration of Human Rights. Article 2 would tell him that "Everyone is entitled to all the rights and freedoms set forth in this Declaration, without distinction of any kind, such as race, colour, sex, language, religion, ... or other status." And Article 3 would have told the murderous rioters in Gujarat, in their choice of Hindi or Gujarati, that “Everyone has the right to life, liberty and security of person.”

    But I dwell on technicalities. Mr Patel must surely mean that it is an education in English—and therefore with western, enlightenment values—that produces smart, rational beings. “The damage is done by a Hindi-medium world view,” he writes, whereas English “opens access to the world outside the tribe”. Without getting into the wisdom of calling the 240m-odd speakers of Hindi (and mutually comprehensible Urdu) or 46m speakers of Gujarati “tribes”, perhaps Mr Patel is getting his causality the wrong way around.

    The Indians who speak English already have access to the outside world. The 2001 census found that a mere 230,000 Indians spoke it as their native tongue. These, presumably, are the people that speak the language well enough to be influenced by the underlying culture that Mr Patel argues in favour of. But to speak English as a native language means going to an expensive private school in a city. The conditions for that ensure that these English-speakers come from wealthy or least upper middle class families. Having money means having security. That, in turn, means you don’t blame your poor neighbours for everything that is wrong with your life.

    In her remarkable new book about life in a Mumbai slum, Katherine Boo, a New Yorker staffer, concludes that the world’s unequal societies don’t explode into violent insurrection because poor people pick on other poor people, not the rich. Just as the wealth flowing into India has yet to trickle down to its very poorest (though it has already lifted hundreds of millions out of poverty), the troubles of the poor leave the better off unaffected. Hundreds of Muslims died in riots in 2002 because poor Hindus expressed their frustrations at day-to-day life against equally poor Muslims. In 1992, when similar riots spread across India, it was the poorest towns and neighbourhoods that became war zones. The nice bits of town remained relatively peaceful.

    The barbaric behaviour seen in Indian riots has everything to do with poverty and despair and nothing to do with language. Columns such as those by Mr Patel in business newspapers for English-speaking city people only blame the poor for not lifting themselves out of poverty, in this case by schooling their children in their own language.

  • Etymology

    Etymology man!

    Feb 3rd 2012, 16:34 by R.L.G. | NEW YORK

    JOHNSON readers have asked for more etymology. Well, that's the opposite of what these hapless folks in need of a superhero called for. From the always excellent xkcd.

  • Journalese

    Headline headaches

    Feb 2nd 2012, 18:41 by R.L.G. | NEW YORK

    GETTING the gist of a news story across in six or so words is difficult, which is one reason editing at a newspaper is a thankless task. (That, and cleaning up the prose, the facts and the argument of a writer who then gets 100% of the byline.) Headsup is one blog largely devoted to this difficult art of headline-writing. And the folks at Language Log have written often about "crash blossoms", headlines that are confusing to analyse because non-content words like articles, prepositions and so forth are left out, leaving a headline that can be read several ways. Many crash blossoms come from Britain, where editors often assume the reader is familiar with the characters and plot of an ongoing story, and so pile up long noun-noun compounds that tell the whole chapter, like "Sex quiz cricket ace in hotel suicide leap".

    Having acknowledged that headline writing is hard, I was surprised to find not one or two but three significantly ambiguous headlines in the Wall Street Journal I read on my phone on my morning commute today. Here goes:

    Unilever Sees Small Profit Rise: Was it a small profit that rose? Or was it a small rise in an otherwise big profit? The first words of the story confirm the second reading, which I figured was the more likely. I probably wouldn't have thought much more about this, but then I read the next one. 

    PetroChina Buys Stake in Shell Canada Asset: To some, this might seem easy, to others not so much. If you follow energy, you will probably have guessed that PetroChina, an energy company, bought an energy asset (a shale-gas prospect) from Shell Canada. But if you follow financial markets, you know that Chinese companies have often bought shells of listed American companies and then merged with them, a controversial "reverse merger" that puts the Chinese company on American markets without pre-IPO vetting. Perhaps since American regulators were cracking down, Chinese companies were buying Canadian shell companies (assets) instead. That was wrong—again, thinking about it for a few extra seconds I guessed correctly. The web headline had the extra word "shale", which would have cleared this up, but my phone did not.  Again, I might have shrugged, but then I read the third.

    Swiss Watch Exports Hit Record High: This was a legitimate 50-50 toss-up. A headline reading "Belgians watch exports hit record high" would have been unambiguous: the Belgians have seen their country's exports hit an all-time record. But it just so happens the Swiss are known for their watchmaking. In boom times for the very rich, have sales of Patek Philippes and Rolexes taken back off?  Take a guess, click through, and see if you were right.

    Many crash blossoms are funny because they invite logically possible but otherwise hilarious readings ("Squad helps dog bite victim"). Others are funny because they are nearly impenetrable ("Bright sparks weather gala night power cut to party on"). A third class of crash blossoms is the possible but neither likely nor funny: just another from the Journal today is "New York Times Profit Falls". Did the city of New York time the plummeting of certain kinds of profits? No, since New York doesn't have the power, nor the incentive, to do this. A newspaper, the New York Times, has seen its profits fall. Most people wouldn't be hung up on this one for long on the wrong reading. 

    But the fourth category, that in question today, might be the rarest of all: a headline, overseen by professional editors, that has two totally plausible and very different real-world meanings, both of which might make stories in the Wall Street Journal. If it hadn't happened three times in a day, I'd have remembered just how hard editing is and given the Journal's folks a pass, but this might warrant a bit of their extra attention. 

  • Sound change

    One more thought on "my plezh"

    Feb 1st 2012, 17:31 by R.L.G. | NEW YORK

    IN LAST week's posting on slang "abbrevs" I noticed that many end in the [zh] or [dzh] sounds ("cazh" for casual and "ledge" for "legend".) I hazarded that these are probably fun for teens because not many natural English words end in these sounds, especially [zh] (ʒ, in the International Phonetic Alphabet).  

    What I didn't notice earlier is that in all the cases I mentioned (cazh/casual, plezh/pleasure, uzhe/usual), the zh-sound is represented by the letter s. This is weird, on the surface. But last year, in describing "the ushe", I wrote 

    In all cases, the following letter is a "u".  In most of those cases, the "u" would have once been pronounced with a bit of a y-sound at the beginning: mez-yoor, trez-yoor, yooz-yual. But do that over and over again, and the y-sound colonises the previous consonant:  make the z-sound and the y-sound back to back, over and over again, and you can see how they join up to become the zh-sound. It saves the tongue the work of moving backwards quickly and precisely. 

    The same thing happens with the letter t, which has a predictable sound, but not in suffixes like "-tion" and "-tial". Followed by an i, which was once pronounced something like a y, the t underwent a similar process as the s in "usual".  Say "ty" over and over again, as if they come between two vowels ("atyon, atyon, atyon"), quickly, over and over again, and you'll probably arrive at "achon" before too long. The t has undergone "lenition" or weakening, going from a stop-consonant  (one that blocks the airflow) to a fricative (which merely slows it down).  From [ch], it's a short road to [sh].

    So a second fun element of a lot of these slang words is that they end in a [ch], [sh] or [zh]-sound that "shouldn't be there", because they're spelled with an s or a t.  (One more example: "claymaish" for "claymation" here; thanks to Andrew Sullivan.)  

    Summary: though the kids couldn't explain it, they've noticed that letters like "t" and "s" undergo weird sound changes when followed by certain sounds. Cutting those words off at those mutated sounds is fun.

  • Spanish in America

    Enough English for public office?

    Jan 30th 2012, 18:46 by R.L.G. | NEW YORK

    ONE of the top stories on the New York Times website last week was that of Alejandrina Cabrera, a Latina citizen of San Luis, Arizona. Ms Cabrera was running for city council, but was stopped by a judge from appearing on the ballot because she doesn't speak fluent enough English. Arizona law requires officeholders to do so. A linguist from Brigham Young University, hired as an expert the court, found that she had "basic survival level" English, and not more. 

    The Times seems to back up the linguist's assessment. She was asked, on the witness stand at her hearing, where she went to high school. She was unable to answer. Her later explanation to the Times, did not help matters: "My brain, my mind was white. That was my first time in court." By "white", though, she meant "blank". Both words are blanco in Spanish. And a city council is a formal public setting; if she was terrified by a courtroom into speechlessness, it is hard to imagine success on the city council.

    Another of her explanations also didn't help her case. She said that the linguist who examined her, an Australian, pronounced "summer" as "summa", and so she thought he meant Somerton, a nearby town. If her grip on the context was so loose that a common foreign accent made her confuse a season with a town, this is on her own shoulders.

    Finally, Ms Cabrera later told CNN en Español (video in Spanish) that she had subsequently had her hearing tested; her doctor found (on a 1-5 scale) that one ear rated only a 2, the other a 3. But she seemed to have no trouble understanding the CNN interviewers over a fuzzy Skype connection. 

    Commenters from the left see skullduggery in the decision to keep Ms Cabrera off the ballot. John McIntyre of the Baltimore Sun asks

    I wonder how a judge would have ruled had he been called upon to decide the qualifications of the elder Richard Daley to be mayor of Chicago on the basis of his mastery of English. 

    Well, you can see Daley here; my guess is that Arizona's Judge Nelson would have let him run for city council. What's more, I don't think Arizona's law is unreasonable. It sounds like Ms Cabrera speaks only basic English, not to a level where voters could trust she could handle her duties in it. I haven't been able to find a video of her speaking English, but I have no reason to distrust the court's expert. 

    Does this mean, as many conservatives fret, that Spanish is taking over regions of the country, even official domains like city councils?  Hardly.  This is one person. She does speak English, merely not well enough, probably because she spent much of her childhood in Mexico. And she was not allowed to run for office in the end. Most importantly, this story from a small border town was rare enough that it made the Times, the Wall Street Journal and CNN. In other words, much more than this will be needed to prove that Spanish poses any real threat to English in America.

  • Readability

    Talking down to America

    Jan 27th 2012, 19:33 by E.G. | AUSTIN

    THOSE of you who have the luck or judgment to ignore the endless stream of non sequiturs surrounding America's 2012 presidential election may have missed an unusual line of comment about Barack Obama's annual State of the Union speech, which he delivered on Tuesday. It started with this post from Eric Ostermeier, at the University of Minnesota's "Smart Politics" web site:

    A Smart Politics study of the 70 orally delivered State of the Union Addresses since 1934 finds the text of Obama's 2012 speech to have tallied the third lowest score on the Flesch-Kincaid readability test, at an 8.4 grade level.

    Bolding and italics his. The president, Mr Ostermeier noted, had promised to keep his message simple. "But was it too simplistic?" he asked. Some of Mr Obama's critics seem to say yes. Here's a typical harrumph: "There's talking down to people, and then, there's condescending." "Knowing our media elite, they would process this information and then praise Obama for talking down to the people," concludes another. One woman put Mitch Daniels' response speech to the same test and found that he ranked at a 14th-grade level. "Governor Daniels was speaking to us as adults!" she says.

    It's impossible to tell whether this dudgeon is highly shared—to be fair, it doesn't seem to have gotten that much traction—but this sort of critique isn't exactly new. For several years Mr Obama has been repeatedly attacked, from the right, for not being as intelligent as Democrats would have people believe. To be more precise, there are a lot of people who angrily object to the idea that Mr Obama might be reasonably good with words. They're appalled that he uses a TelePrompter. Every time he misspeaks, they pounce. In some cases...well, let's hear it from one of them: "The public is asked to believe Obama wrote Dreams From My Father on his own, almost as though he were some sort of literary idiot savant." This latest line of attack is more of the same. The question of why some of Mr Obama's critics are so fixated on his rhetorical skills is one for Democracy in America.

    But as this is the language blog, let's take the complaint at face value. Mr Obama's speech was relatively simple, more simple than most State of the Union addresses. However, there's no normative weight to the Flesch-Kincaid grade level. The score is a function of how long the sentences are and how many syllables the words have. It's a weak proxy for accessibility, not substance or value. I just tested a couple of recent articles in The Economist—which I hope we can all agree is a reasonably well-written publication—and found grade levels of 10.3, 10.6, and 10.8. George Orwell's "Why I Write": 9.5. "The Love Song of J. Alfred Prufrock": 7.4, suggesting that the Fleisch-Kincaid formula isn't that sensitive to context. In any case, such comparisons are a little silly; no one judges political speeches on their syntactic complexity. (Reagan's address to the nation after the Challenger disaster: 5.7.)

    No one, that is, except Mr Obama's critics. It's notably that he's also caught flak for speaking at too high of a grade level; after his speech on the BP oil spill registered a 9.8, he was dinged for being too "professorial."  As my colleague noted then, the Flesch-Kincaid score is a "mindless bit of math," insensitive to meaning or intention. If anyone is condescending to the voters here, it's probably Mr Ostermeier, who suggests in his post that the president should speak to Congress at a higher level than the people. As for the voters themselves, the silver lining of this little flap is that it evinces such a high demand for advanced literacy that Mr Obama's bold proposal to ensure that more kids graduate from high school might get some traction.

  • Accents

    Where did that Spanish theta come from (and where did it go)?

    Jan 25th 2012, 21:44 by R.L.G. | NEW YORK

    ANOTHER question from readers: Why does the "theta" sound in Spanish Spanish not prevail in Latin America?  For those unfamiliar, the letters c (when followed by e or i) and z (always) are pronounced like the English "th" as in "thin", in Spain. (It's often called theta after the Greek letter θしーた, which is pronounced the same way.) My favorite example is civilización, which comes out thivilithathion in Spanish Spanish but not in Latin America. Nate in Phoenix, Arizona asked if this was because, as he had heard, Spanish settlers in Latin America came from the south of Spain (Andalucia), where the theta is not used? 

    I didn't know the answer, so wrote to John McWhorter, an expert in language contact and change.  His reply:

    While theta happened in Castille, some of Andalusia developed into just /s/ while other places there, and elsewhere, had an opposition between /s/ and an apicoalveolar /s'/ between /s/ and theta. Both of the latter are still around in Latin America, as opposed to just /s/, and thus the idea that Latin America has /s/ from Andalusia fails from both sides of the pond. But it's true that there's no theta in Latin America because Castillians didn't come over much.

    So Nate's conjecture was almost but not quite right.

  • Slang

    The abbrevs are my plezh

    Jan 23rd 2012, 20:51 by R.L.G. | NEW YORK

    FOR those alarmed at yesterday's peeving, today's post will be a celebration of teenage slang. A few posts in the linguablogosphere have recently checked in on teenage shortenings: Stan Carey ("Ledgebag is totes amaze") here, and Ben Yagoda ("Totes cray-cray abbrevs") here. Mr Carey is broadly admiring, Mr Yagoda slightly dismayed. 

    Mr Carey's post got me noticing that many of these fanciful abbrevs end in consonants pronounced in the same place— they're "voiced postalveolar fricatives", with the tongue behind the alveolar ridge and the vocal cords vibrating. Here they are, with International Phonetic Alphabet symbols, and an English approximation:

    ʒ or "zh": plezh ("my plezh", my pleasure), uzhe ("the uzhe", the usual), cazh ("totes cazh", very casual)

    dʒ or "dzh":  ledge ("legend"), dodge ("dodgy"), tradge ("tragic")

    Maybe cutting words off at these sounds is a little more common because the results are simply funny. Not that many English words end in ʒ or dʒ, and many of those that do have a bit of a silly ring: cadge, smidge, smudge, drudge, hodge-podge, fridge and such sit alongside a few respectable words like bridge and ridge and dredge.  Even fewer words end in ʒ than in dʒ, and tend to be French borrowings or produced by the -age suffix borrowed from Old French: mirage, dressage, frottage, arbitrage...

    So my guess is that it's fun to end a word in ʒ or dʒ just because English doesn't do so very often. As for Mr Yagoda's dismay, I just can't share it. Teens play with the language not out of a desire for efficiency ("some of these 'abbreviations' aren't even shorter!") but because it's fun.

  • Peeves

    Issues with "issues"

    Jan 23rd 2012, 20:20 by R.L.G. | NEW YORK

    SEVERAL commenters have detected what they decry as a drift towards prescriptivism here at Johnson.  Well, guilty as charged: we all think some ways of writing and speaking are better than others, and a language blog that never criticised anything would be unusual. We've also been relentlessly descriptivist, bringing usage facts to bear on peeves we feel are unjustifiable. Johnson, like The Economist, has time for both facts and opinions.

    But back to complaining.  One commenter asked, in our "What should we write about?" post, about the word "issue", used nowadays often for "problem". We don't like the drift. 

    "Issue" seems to have migrated in an unusual direction, from official euphemism right down to the kids.  Beginning with "Our servers are currently experiencing outage issues," it's now unremarkable to hear a teenaged girl saying "I have an issue with you."

    And "issue"-as-"problem" seems here to stay. The first results for "I have an issue with" in Google include things like "a barking dog", "a website that is potentially posting copyrighted material", "activating MS office 2010" and the like. Many are technical. A problem is a type of issue, of course: in politics we talk about the Iranian nuclear issue, the unemployment issue, the Israel-Palestinian issue. But an issue did not always need to be negative. The economy is an issue in boom times, too, even if a less salient one. 

    But our style book, in a ruling this Johnson endorses, wishes that people with "issues" might just come out with it:

    "The Economist has issues–51 a year–but if you think you have issues with The Economist, you probably mean you have complaints, irritations or delivery problems. If you disagree with The Economist, you may take issue with it. Be precise."

    If your issue is a problem, and you want to bring it up with someone, it might be a good idea to go ahead and use the P-word. This doesn't have to be a cause for unpleasantness, but it does communicate your feelings and expectations more pointedly than "issue". Get it out there.

    (But if you do have problems with your delivery, comments here are not the best way to get them addressed.)

  • Importing words

    My Godji!

    Jan 20th 2012, 10:09 by H.J. | SÃO PAULO

    A SHORT blogpost from Brazil in response to my colleague-Johnson’s musings on mixing languages. As he mentioned, I’ve posted on this before, when I asked readers what word they’d most like to import into English. This time I’m going to tell a story that works the other way round.

    A couple of weeks ago the Brazilian television network Band started a “reality” programme called Mulheres Ricas (Rich Women): you can read another foreign correspondent’s take on it here. There’s lots to say about it from the sociological point of view, of course, but at the São Paulo foreign-correspondent’s monthly get-together a few days after the first episode aired, the most-discussed aspect was one of the participants’ penchant for sprinkling the word “hello” throughout her spoken Portuguese. Val Marchiori didn’t use it in any way I’ve heard English speakers use it—to greet people, to express surprise, or as a sort of verbal raised eyebrow or shrugged shoulder. She seemed to be inserting it randomly, I think because to her it sounded sophisticated. 

    In the second episode the following week, the “hellos” continued—with a new addition: “My Godji”. Portuguese words don't end in -d or -t often, but rather -de and -te. Brazilian speakers pronounce these "-ji" and "-chi". When they import words that end in d or t, they usually pronounce them as if they ended in “de” or “te”—that is, ending with a “ji” or “chi” sound. Hence, “My Godji”. So much more expressive than the original, it’s my new favourite exclamation—and I’ve already heard other English-speakers in São Paulo using it too. Could be the next big trend?

  • Accent

    Henry Higgins in 2012

    Jan 19th 2012, 14:29 by R.L.G. | LONDON

    BACK in Britain for a few days, and often the capital city doesn't feel very British, from the Russians at the hotel to the Italians at Pret to the American nonsense on television. It's good when you still find a nice old British tradition, then: a warm and flavourful ale, a black cab with a brilliant driver, or a newspaper article shot through with unconscious language prejudice. 

    Today's article in the Independent is about the rise in people taking elocution lessons. Mind, this probably wouldn't make the business section, as the "rise" is documented by one tutor whose business is booming. But if a true trend, it would be interesting to hear that more people are taking classes to learn to speak differently. (The boom is said to be fuelled by anxious job-seekers in a weak economy.) My complaint is the constant refrain that people are aiming to "lose their accents": "Since Annette Burgess began her elocution lessons last autumn, she feels she has made huge strides and has ambitions to discard her accent completely," and so on, several times.

    Not that the journalist or his sources are Henry Higgins-style snobs:

    In what we like to think of as an increasingly classless society, and at a time when the distinctive regional accents are gradually being melded and lost, it seems a shame that there are so many people anxious to lose their accents. "I get a lot of requests from people looking to reduce their regional accents, Midwinter says. "I think as long as people speak clearly, if they have an accent, that's OK, as long as they can be understood."

    But there it is, "lose their accents" again. The mental frame is that the speech of Devon is an "accent", an encumbrance to be lost, while someone who uses the Received Pronunciation native to southeastern England has "no accent". Only the mute have no accent. When I fly home, the first thing I'll notice at the airport is that General American is an accent, too, unless I get an earful of New York English.

    The saddest thing is to see people turn the prejudice on themselves: 

    "I also wanted lessons to help me do away with my Devonian accent. I originally come from Plymouth, so there is a Devon twang to some of my speech that I would like to lose. I felt it was holding me back in terms of forward progression within my career."

    Don't let the snobs get you down!  By all means, anyone who wishes should to take lessons to enunciate clearly, project, slow down, choose words effectively and conquer the common fear of public speaking. But a clever argument charmingly delivered—as I can attest from talking to colleagues who don't speak in RP—will always carry the day.

    I understand journalistic shorthand. But this article reads very differently—correctly, not just compassionately—if the first mention of "losing an accent" were replaced with "learning the Received Pronunciation of southeastern England", and every subsequent mention with "learning RP". Then the picture would become clear: some people feel they must learn a pronunciation not native to them in order to meet others' prejudices. That's quite a different story, one that would be better at home in the left-leaning Independent.

  • Mixing languages

    Qué es ese code-switching?

    Jan 18th 2012, 18:33 by R.L.G. | LONDON

    THANKS for the many comments on the last post, suggesting topics (and keep them coming).  

    Omulu and Human Child asked a few related questions about mixing languages. This happens at several different levels, for different reasons.  Omulu asked about untranslatable words like the oft-cited German GemütlichkeitGemütlichkeit is a kind of barroom cosiness with good friends, Bratkartoffeln and a nice local beer, or maybe a family dinner with intimate conversation and a good few laughs at the holidays. The Dutch have a similar word, gezellig, an adjective. The Danes like to call things in this category hyggelig. Hyggelig, like the others, is common, it's laden with associations, and it doesn't have an easy one-for-one English equivalent, so I hear my Danish mother-in-law calling things "cosy" all the time when I know she means hyggelig. I, for one, knowing cosy doesn't cover it, often say things are hyggelig to my wife, even when we're speaking English. This is one of the perks and joys of language-learning. My colleague posted a while back asking people to name words they'd like to import from foreign languages into English, and got quite a few answers.

    Then there's another level of this: borrowing completely ordinary words from another language for play, because the other language is prestigious, or for no good reason at all. There is no reason to say Weltanschauung for "worldview", unless you just don't get enough chances to type two u's in succession and have tired of writing about vacuums. Many foreigners borrow English words like this these days, and it drives purists who speak those languages crazy pointing out the perfectly good native substitutes.  Probably my favorite is the handful of European languages that have borrowed "baby": das Baby in German for example. (The word Säugling, cognate to "suckling", is now quaint or old-fashioned.) 

    But "code-switching", which Human Child brought up, is quite a different thing. Linguists use this off-putting term to describe people's switching back and forth between two dialects or languages they speak well, quickly and often. Some speakers of black English can put on their most buttoned-up English and then quickly switch to black dialect, like this reporter. (Warning: hilarious spontaneous profanity.  To those who call him "ghetto", I'd respond that he's just bidialectal.) Many Latinos in New York seem to do it almost randomly as they speak, as in the title of this paper on the subject: "Sometimes I'll start a sentence in Spanish y termino en español." Some linguists claim to see predictability in it; that the switches tend to happen more often at clause boundaries, for example, or that people switch to their first or home language when talking about more emotional topics. Those interested in more should click through to the paper and poke around the footnotes. Code-switching has many varieties and many motivations.

    Finally, there's just plain language-mixing, like the portuñol that happens where Brazil and its Spanish-speaking neighbors meet.  Languages on semi-equal footing coming into contact on a daily basis can spawn a partly improvised, partly stable halfway-tongue.  If the languages aren't quite on equal footing, you can get intermediate results like the Saxon-Viking-Norman mix that produced English, still basically the Saxon language but with heavy input from French and Old Norse.

    Language-contact is one of those things that is fascinating to linguists and infurating to some purists and nationalists. It's a fact of life, though, and I'm more in the "fascinated" camp, even if I don't agree with your Weltanschauung.

  • Reader response

    Ask the audience: What should Johnson write about?

    Jan 17th 2012, 21:40 by R.L.G. | NEW YORK

    READERS have, in the past, been responsible for some of our most interesting posts by simply asking a question. k.a.gardner's recent question about the comma-splice led to a post. So given that today is a travel day and there's no time to write here (not to mention yesterday's American holiday), help a Johnson out. What would you like to see us answer? 

  • Names

    Moniker madness

    Jan 13th 2012, 18:07 by R.L.G. | NEW YORK

    WATCHING college (American) football this bowl season, I was hoping my Georgia Bulldogs' free safety would return for his last season next year. (For those unaccustomed, college athletes may play only for four years total in America. Star prospects often leave after three or even two years if they are good enough to join the professional leagues.) Not only was he Georgia's best defensive player, but it was endless fun hearing his name: Bacarri Rambo.  The good news? He'll be back next year. Improbable fun fact?  His original name was Bacarri Fudge

    Today, I learn of the Name of the Year contest, via Nancy Friedman. I'd never heard of it, but now I'm hooked. In short, the website lets readers vote on their favourite peculiar personal names, in a knockout-style tournament. Who will be the name of the year?  We're in the round of 16 already, so you can choose from Rockwell Bonecutter, Leviticus Payne, Ebenezer Noonoo, Courvoisier Winetavius Richardson, Chuntania Dangerfield, Delorean Blow, Solo Alone, Yolanda Supersad, RexAchilles Imperial, Vernon Lee Bad Marriage Jr., Monsterville Horton IV, Taco B.M. Monster, La'Peaches Pitts, Neptune Pringle III, Heidi Hohl and Madz Negro.  Like Ms Friedman, I'm going to be voting for Courvoisier Winetavius Richardson to the end.  (In case you're suspicious, as I initially was, the Name of the Year editors have verified that these are all real people.) 

    To be serious for a moment: many such names belong to black Americans. It has been a cause of both mirth and puzzlement for the more traditionally named. It's not hard to understand why those cut off for centuries from full American citizenship and human dignity might not choose Dennis or Steve for their children. Many blacks have reached for Swahili influences in choosing names, though most black Americans' ancestors came from western Africa, not Swahili's heartland on the other side of the continent. But many names are either borrowed from unusual sources (brand names like Courvoisier) or made up (Chuntania)—nothing African about either. And I can verify that I have seen an African-American checkout clerk in New Orleans whose name-tag read "Bellowney", and in the same store, a Sayonara. 

    Experiments with identical résumés sent to employers, one with a distinctively black name and one with a white-sounding one, have found (surely to no one's surprise) that a DeShawn is less likely to get an interview than a Michael with the same qualifications. Racism abides. But Steven Levitt, in a paper described in his book "Freakonomics", analysed real-world people born in California. He found that those with distinctly black names fared poorly because they came from demographic groups (with poor parents, poor education) that predicted future poverty. Regression analysis found no effect from the names themselves.

    So what's in a name?  Variety, individuality, culture, family, history and, sometimes, creativity. There's nothing racist at having a smile upon finding a Monsterville or a Chuntania. There's something pleasingly biblical about a hard-hitting Leviticus Payne (another American football player). Rockwell Bonecutter is proof that unusual monikers aren't limited to any race. Taco Monster, PhD, a Dutchman, shows that even if Americans might think you're the Cookie Monster's Mexican cousin, you can still make it as an epidemiologist. So Vernon Lee Bad Marriage, Jr. (convicted of assaulting his girlfriend) and Courvoisier Winetavius Richardson (accused of a string of bank robberies): whatever's in a name, don't blame your parents. Fates like these are your own to choose.

  • Grammar

    Switched

    Jan 12th 2012, 19:34 by G.L. | NEW YORK

    More switches. Less hitches

    ONE could just put this picture in the what-is-the-world-coming-to file and leave it at that. But the more I think about it, the more I wonder whether it was a mere infelicity on the part of an ill-educated copywriter for New York's Metropolitan Transity Authority, or a calculated act.

    As Mark Liberman and Geoff Pullum explain, it's far from true that "fewer" must always be used with plural nouns; "less" is correct when the noun refers to something divisible, as in "seven dollars less", and may be allowed even with something indivisible when it's being considered as part of a "mass-like quantity" (for instance, "a margin of 5,000 votes or less"). So could hitches be considered a mass-like quantity? Well, maybe, if you think of them as contributing to some aggregate quotient of unhappiness in the life of the New York commuter. But I somehow doubt the copywriter looked up Messrs Liberman and Pullum before brushing the cake crumbs off his or her keyboard and getting down to work.

    So that leaves two more explanations, besides sheer ignorance. The first is that the writer felt that "New switches, fewer hitches" just didn't scan as well. Personally I think it scans just fine. "Newer switches, fewer hitches" would be perfect, of course, but I suspect the MTA wouldn't want its switches to be called merely "newer", as if to imply that they bought a second-hand job lot off the London Underground and had them shipped across the Atlantic, to replace the decidedly long-in-the-tooth ones bequeathed as a gift from the Paris Metro back in the 1970s.

    And the last option is just that "less" is the way people talk, and the MTA doesn't want to appear snooty and out-of-touch. Which would be a shame, because a message that stares you in the face on your way to and from work every day is bound to make an impression, and maybe it's paternalistic of me, but I think that public authorities (unlike, say, entertainers) ought to take an educational stance, not a populist one.

    But I'm curious what you think. Was this a deliberate error, or an accidental one? And if deliberate, was it justified?

     

  • Obscenity

    Free speech for prudes and plutocrats

    Jan 10th 2012, 22:21 by R.L.G. | NEW YORK

    AMERICA'S Supreme Court heard a case about obscenity on broadcast television today. The New York Times notes that while the hearing was unusually lively, the judges don't look likely to overturn the authority past courts have given the government to fine a broadcaster—up to hundreds of thousands of dollars—for a brief obscenity.

    What amazes me is the plastic definition of free speech invoked by one justice in particular.

    Justice Antonin Scalia, who in other settings has been hostile to government regulation of speech, said there was value in holding the line here.

    “This has a symbolic value,” he said, “just as we require a certain modicum of dress for the people that attend this court.”

    “These are public airwaves,” Justice Scalia went on, adding: “I’m not sure it even has to relate to juveniles, to tell you the truth.”

    By "in other settings...hostile to government regulation of speech", the reporter means that Justice Scalia believes it to be an unconstitutional restriction of free speech for governments to limit vast spending by corporations and unions to influence elections.  The Citizens United decision is rightly a controversial one. But if a jurist is going to be a free-speech purist, I'd prefer that he see it through. By what logic is Bono's "fucking brilliant" (5:44 in the video, one of the cases that has engaged the Supreme Court) a threat to the republic, while a SuperPAC is not?

    Addendum:  I can't resist promoting teacup775's comment: "Now if Bono founds a SuperPAC that touts a candidate as fucking brilliant, we can hope to hear Scalia's brilliant reasoning as to why that speech is not protected."  Actually, I expect that if this should happen, Justice Scalia would find himself on the side of grudgingly protecting it. Maybe Stephen Colbert's SuperPAC can provide the test case. 

  • Punctuation

    The dreaded comma splice

    Jan 10th 2012, 16:57 by R.L.G. | NEW YORK

    SEVERAL months ago I was surprised to see Arnold Zwicky, a linguist, use a comma splice. A few commenters took me to task for being over-picky. The question came up again in the comments several days ago, when k.a.gardner, a frequent commenter, asked for a post on the comma splice. One of my colleagues quickly replied that "The comma-splice rule is totally arbitrary," and a back-and-forth ensued.

    What is a comma splice?  Prof Zwicky wrote back in July

    "this is not even a tempest in a teapot, it’s a fuss in a thimbleful of spit."

    That's two independent clauses joined only by a comma, or a comma splice, sometimes called a "comma fault".  Ashbird, another commenter, was taught as I was: the comma-splice is an error.  My senior English teacher marked down any paper with even a single comma splice by two letter grades, so that an otherwise perfect A paper would receive a C. (She applied the same rule for fragments and run-on sentences.)  My colleague, however, says it's a matter of style.  The Economist doesn't have a ruling on comma splices in the style book, but I don't recall ever having seen one in the newspaper. And I would notice; my English teacher's injunction gave me a terror-loathing of comma splices that has never left me.

    But, as ever, there are facts to be had, and in cases like this, the go-to reference is the Merriam-Webster Dictionary of English Usage. (It is hard to praise this book enough.) Sure enough, the comma splice was once part of the best English usage:

    As to the old one, I knew not what to do with him, he was so fierce I durst not go into the pit to him — (Daniel Defoe, Robinson Crusoe, 1719)

    The New Jersey job was obtained, I contrived a copperplate press for it — (Benjamin Franklin, Autobiography, 1771)

    By the nineteenth century, MWDEU gives examples by Lord Byron, Jane Austin and Lewis Carroll, but only in personal letters. This trend continued into the 20th century, where Ronald Reagan and E.B. White both used comma splices in letters. (Yes, that is the E.B. White who put his name, in "The Elements of Style", to this crisp injunction: "Do not join independent clauses with a comma," though "Elements" allows for rare exceptions if the sentences are very short and closely related.)

    By the 20th century, rulebooks commonly warned against the comma splice, with the effect that it now seems limited to informal writing such as letters, or in reported speech.

    The Ambassador...responded with a blast of enthusiasm. "Those weren't tough questions, those were kid-glove questions..." — John Updike, Bech is Back, 1982

    MWDEU, which often debunks sticklerish rules with massed evidence from indisputably great writers, says "uncorrected examples are so hard to find in print" that "You should not try the device [of the comma splice] unless you are very sure of what you want it to accomplish."

    I agree. Editors have made the comma splice so rare that they leap off the page (unpleasantly so, for me) when I spot one. The comma splice is unnecessary; a brief pause between two related thoughts can be accomplished by a semicolon like the one in this sentence. A full stop separates two thoughts more cleanly. Unless you're being aphoristic ("Man proposes, God disposes") or intentionally seeking a loose-knit style, beware that a comma splice is probably not worth the readers it will irritate. 

    Update: Stan Carey, in a thoughtful post with lots of evidence, was softer on the comma splice than I am. He also reminds me that "Elements of Style" allows for limited comma splicing, so I added a bit to this effect above.

  • Scope

    Unless what is otherwise specified?

    Jan 9th 2012, 18:39 by R.L.G. | NEW YORK

    OUT last weekend with friends at a cool new burger restaurant in Brooklyn, I was taken aback by this sentence, which appeared on the menu above the many different burgers below.

    "Burgers are served with organic beef unless otherwise specified."

    Anyone else surprised by this?  Some of my dinner companions were surprised like I was; others read it with no trouble.

  • Lexical accuracy

    The failure of American political speech

    Jan 6th 2012, 21:53 by G.L. | NEW YORK

    OUTRIGHT abuse of the word "socialism" is one of the few things about America that really peeves me. (By "really" I mean a visceral, principled peeve, not the grumpy, petty kind of peeve about how hard it is to get a decent cup of tea.) As our Book of isms says, socialism is

    A political and economic theory that holds that the means of production and distribution in an economy should be owned or regulated by the community as a whole or by a central government.

    Got that? The means of production. Owned* by the government. As in the "Union of Soviet Socialist Republics". Not Communist Republics, because they never actually attained their idyllic goal of common ownership of all property. Socialism is not "the government should provide healthcare" or "the rich should be taxed more" nor any of the other watery social-democratic positions that the American right likes to demonise by calling them "socialist"—and granted, it is chiefly the right that does so, but the fact that rightists are so rarely confronted and ridiculed for it means that they have successfully muddied the political discourse to the point where an awful lot of Americans have only the flimsiest grasp of what socialism is. And that, in a country that sent tens of thousands of men to die fighting socialism, is frankly an insult to those dead soldiers' memories.

    With that off my chest, it's therefore interesting to read our fellow blogger Will Wilkinson's post on another blog on his problems with the word "libertarian", a label frequently applied to him. Given his views on taxation, the state, redistributive policies and so on, he can only, he says, be called a liberal, or maybe a "liberaltarian". Once again, as he points out, both "liberal" and "libertarian" are frequently misunderstood in much the same way that "socialist" is.

    Similar confusion, writes Mark Lilla in the current New York Review of Books, has befallen the word "conservative". Tracing the genesis of the term in its modern sense to Edmund Burke in the aftermath of the French Revolution, he writes that conservatives, of whom Burke was one,

    have always seen society as a kind of inheritance we receive and are responsible for; we have obligations toward those who came before and to those who will come after, and these obligations take priority over our rights... Classical liberals like John Stuart Mill, in contrast to conservatives, give individuals priority over society, on anthropological as well as moral grounds. They assume that societies are genuinely constructs of human freedom, that whatever we inherit from them, they can always be unmade or remade through free human action.  

    By these lights, as Mr Lilla points out, Americans are liberal at heart:

    We take it for granted that we are born free, that we constitute society, it doesn’t constitute us, and that together we legitimately govern ourselves. Most intellectuals who call themselves conservatives today accept as self-evident the truths enumerated in the Declaration of Independence, which no traditional European conservative could.

    And yet "liberal" is almost a pejorative in America, tainted as it is with associations to that demon-word, "socialist". When people here own up to being liberals, they have to do it with a certain defiance.

    I don't think this is a matter simply of linguistic drift or the mutation that political terms undergo when they cross the Atlantic. "Words are failing us," Mr Lilla writes, and I agree. The cause seems, at least to me, fairly obvious. People tend to use these labels more about their opponents than they do about themselves. The purpose of the label is not to describe someone but to classify him, to put him in the "enemy" box, and that makes playing fast and loose with the meaning of the word practically unavoidable.

    Why this feels more pronounced in America than elsewhere, I'm not sure; it's tempting to blame the increasingly tribal nature of American politics, but I don't have enough time in this country to judge how true that is. Within this framework, though, it bears noting that "libertarian" has not acquired quite the pejorative tinge that some other terms have, no doubt because people with libertarian tendences frequently find themselves on both sides of the political fence.

    * Update: or controlled/managed; "regulated" in this context obviously doesn't mean simply "being subject to government regulation", as a couple of commenters have read it, because every government, no matter how free-market, regulates the economy to some extent.

About Johnson

In this blog, named after the dictionary-maker Samuel Johnson, our correspondents write about the effects that the use (and sometimes abuse) of language have on politics, society and culture around the world

Advertisement

Trending topics

Read comments on the site's most popular topics

Advertisement

Latest blog posts - All times are GMT
Super for democracy?
From Democracy in America - 1 hrs 55 mins ago
The kingmaker is dead
From Baobab - 1 hrs 49 mins ago
Life on the Phillips curve
From Free exchange - 1 hrs 9 mins ago
Few words, many pictures
From Prospero - 3 hrs 43 mins ago
Foul play
From Americas view - February 20th, 11:54
Stop the Linsanity?
From Game theory - February 20th, 8:46
More from our blogs »
Products & events
Stay informed today and every day

Subscribe to The Economist's free e-mail newsletters and alerts.


Subscribe to The Economist's latest article postings on Twitter


See a selection of The Economist's articles, events, topical videos and debates on Facebook.