(Translated by https://www.hiragana.jp/)
Pulse (music): Difference between revisions - Wikipedia Jump to content

Pulse (music): Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
AnomieBOT (talk | contribs)
m Dating maintenance tags: {{Cfn}}
Hyacinth (talk | contribs)
top: -{{cfn|date=October 2018}}, no explanation or reason given
Line 2: Line 2:
[[Image:Pulse wide wave.svg|thumb|200px|A repeating pulse represented graphically as on an [[oscilloscope]].]]
[[Image:Pulse wide wave.svg|thumb|200px|A repeating pulse represented graphically as on an [[oscilloscope]].]]


In [[music]] and [[music theory]], the '''pulse''' consists of [[beat (music)|beat]]s{{cfn|date=October 2018}}<ref name="Winiold">Winold, Allen (1975). "Rhythm in Twentieth-Century Music", ''Aspects of Twentieth-Century Music''. Wittlich, Gary (ed.). Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall. {{ISBN|0-13-049346-5}}.</ref> in a (repeating) of identical yet distinct [[Frequency|periodic]] short-duration [[stimulus (physiology)|stimuli]] perceived as points in [[time]]<ref name="Winiold" /> occurring at the [[mensural level]]. "This pulse is typically what listeners entrain to as they tap their foot or dance along with a piece of music (Handel, 1989), and is also colloquially termed the 'beat,' or more technically the 'tactus' (Lerdahl & Jackendoff, 1983)."<ref>Fitch, W. Tecumseh and Rosenfeld, Andrew J. (2007). "Perception and Production of Syncopated Rhythms", p.44, ''Music Perception'', Vol. 25, Issue 1, pp. 43–58, ISSN 0730-7829.</ref> "Even a person untrained in music, can generally sense the pulse and may respond by tapping a foot or clapping."<ref>Benward, Bruce and Saker, Marilyn (2003). ''Music: In Theory and Practice'', Vol. I, p.9. 7th edition. McGraw-Hill. {{ISBN|0-07-294262-2}}.</ref>
In [[music]] and [[music theory]], the '''pulse''' consists of [[beat (music)|beat]]s<ref name="Winiold">Winold, Allen (1975). "Rhythm in Twentieth-Century Music", ''Aspects of Twentieth-Century Music''. Wittlich, Gary (ed.). Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall. {{ISBN|0-13-049346-5}}.</ref> in a (repeating) of identical yet distinct [[Frequency|periodic]] short-duration [[stimulus (physiology)|stimuli]] perceived as points in [[time]]<ref name="Winiold" /> occurring at the [[mensural level]]. "This pulse is typically what listeners entrain to as they tap their foot or dance along with a piece of music (Handel, 1989), and is also colloquially termed the 'beat,' or more technically the 'tactus' (Lerdahl & Jackendoff, 1983)."<ref>Fitch, W. Tecumseh and Rosenfeld, Andrew J. (2007). "Perception and Production of Syncopated Rhythms", p.44, ''Music Perception'', Vol. 25, Issue 1, pp. 43–58, ISSN 0730-7829.</ref> "Even a person untrained in music, can generally sense the pulse and may respond by tapping a foot or clapping."<ref>Benward, Bruce and Saker, Marilyn (2003). ''Music: In Theory and Practice'', Vol. I, p.9. 7th edition. McGraw-Hill. {{ISBN|0-07-294262-2}}.</ref>


==Definitions==
==Definitions==

Revision as of 01:35, 14 October 2019

A repeating pulse represented graphically as on an oscilloscope.

In music and music theory, the pulse consists of beats[1] in a (repeating) of identical yet distinct periodic short-duration stimuli perceived as points in time[1] occurring at the mensural level. "This pulse is typically what listeners entrain to as they tap their foot or dance along with a piece of music (Handel, 1989), and is also colloquially termed the 'beat,' or more technically the 'tactus' (Lerdahl & Jackendoff, 1983)."[2] "Even a person untrained in music, can generally sense the pulse and may respond by tapping a foot or clapping."[3]

Definitions

Metric levels: beat level shown in middle with division levels above and multiple levels below.
Simple quadruple drum pattern, rock drum kit. Despite the presence of eighth notes, there is a quarter note beat. Play

The pulse may be audible or implied. The tempo of the piece is the speed of the pulse. If a pulse becomes too fast it would become a drone; one that is too slow would be perceived as unconnected sounds.[4] When the period of any continuous beat is faster than 8–10 per second or slower than 1 per 1.5–2 seconds, it cannot be perceived as such.[4] "Musical" pulses are generally specified in the range 40 to 240 beats per minute. The pulse is not necessarily the fastest or the slowest component of the rhythm but the one that is perceived as basic. This is currently most often designated as a crotchet or quarter note when written (see time signature).

Pulse groups

Clear quarter note pulse in 4
4
at a tempo of quarter note=120 Play. At quarter note=600 the pulse becomes a drone Play, while at quarter note=30 the pulse becomes disconnected sounds Play.

While ideal pulses are identical, when pulses are variously accented, this produces two- or three-pulse pulse groups such as strong-weak and strong-weak-weak[1] and any longer group may be broken into such groups of two and three. In fact there is a natural tendency to perceptually group or differentiate an ideal pulse in this way. A repetitive, regularly accented pulse-group is called a metre.

Varied pulse groups equals non-isochronal multiple level Play.

Pulses can occur at multiple metric levels - see figure. Pulse groups may be distinguished as synchronous, if all pulses on slower levels coincide with those on faster levels, and nonsynchronous, if not.

An isochronal or equally spaced pulse on one level that uses varied pulse groups (rather than just one pulse group the whole piece) create a pulse on the (slower) multiple level that is non-isochronal (a stream of 2+3... at the eighth note level would create a pulse of a quarter note+dotted quarter note as its multiple level).

See also

Sources

  1. ^ a b c Winold, Allen (1975). "Rhythm in Twentieth-Century Music", Aspects of Twentieth-Century Music. Wittlich, Gary (ed.). Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall. ISBN 0-13-049346-5.
  2. ^ Fitch, W. Tecumseh and Rosenfeld, Andrew J. (2007). "Perception and Production of Syncopated Rhythms", p.44, Music Perception, Vol. 25, Issue 1, pp. 43–58, ISSN 0730-7829.
  3. ^ Benward, Bruce and Saker, Marilyn (2003). Music: In Theory and Practice, Vol. I, p.9. 7th edition. McGraw-Hill. ISBN 0-07-294262-2.
  4. ^ a b P. Fraisse, Les Structures Rhythmiques, Erasme Paris 1956, H Woodrow Time Perception in "A Handbook of Experimental Psychology", ed. S.S. Stevens, Wiley, NY 1951, both quoted at http://www.zeuxilogy.home.ro/media/manifesto.pdf (zeuxilogy.home.ro)